Peacock| Knowledge About Peacock| About Peacock| NaturWild
Peacock is a common
name for three bird species in the genera Pavo and Afropavo of
the family Phasianidae, the pheasants and their allies. Male peafowl are
referred to as peacocks, and female peafowl are referred to
as peahens, even though peafowl of either sex are often referred
to colloquially as "peacocks".
The two Asiatic species are
the blue or Indian peafowl originally of the Indian subcontinent,
and the green peafowl of Southeast Asia; the one African species is
the Congo peafowl, native only to the Congo Basin. Male peafowl are
known for their piercing calls and their extravagant plumage. The latter is
especially prominent in the Asiatic species, which have an eye-spotted
"tail" or "train" of covert feathers, which they
display as part of a courtship ritual.
The functions of the
elaborate iridescent colouration and large "train" of
peacocks have been the subject of extensive scientific debate. Charles
Darwin suggested that they served to attract females, and the showy
features of the males had evolved by sexual selection. More
recently, Amotz Zahavi proposed in his handicap theory that
these features acted as honest signals of the males' fitness since less fit males would be disadvantaged by the difficulty of surviving with such large and conspicuous structures.
The Indian peacock has iridescent blue and green plumage, mostly metallic blue and green,
but the green peacock has green and bronze body feathers. In both species,
females are a little smaller than males in terms of weight and wingspan, but
males are significantly longer due to the "tail", also known as a
"train". The peacock train consists not of tail quill feathers,
but highly elongated upper tail coverts. These feathers are marked with
eyespots, best seen when a peacock fans his tail. Both sexes of all species
have a crest atop the head. The Indian peahen has a mixture of dull grey,
brown, and green in her plumage. The female also displays her plumage to ward
off female competition or signal danger to her young.
Green peafowl differs from
Indian peafowl in that the male has green and gold plumage and black wings with
a sheen of blue. Unlike Indian peafowl, the green peahen is similar to the
male but has shorter upper tail coverts, a more coppery neck, and overall less
iridescence.
The Congo peacock male does
not display his covert feathers but uses his actual tail feathers during
courtship displays. These feathers are much shorter than those of the Indian
and green species, and the ocelli are much less pronounced. Females of the
Indian and African species are dull grey and/or brown.
Chicks of both sexes in all the species are cryptically coloured. They vary between yellow and tawny, usually with patches of darker brown or light tan and "dirty white" ivory.
Colour and
pattern variations
Hybrids between Indian peafowl
and Green peafowl are called Spaldings after the first person to
successfully hybridise them, Mrs Keith Spalding. Unlike many hybrids,
Spalding are fertile and generally benefit from hybrid vigour; Spalding with
a high-green phenotype do much better in cold temperatures than the
cold-intolerant green peafowl while still looking like their green parents.
Plumage varies between individual Spalding, with some looking far more like
green peafowl and some looking far more like blue peafowl, though most visually
carry traits of both.
In addition to the wild-type
"blue" colouration, several hundred variations in colour and pattern
are recognised as separate morphs of the Indian Blue among peafowl breeders.
Pattern variations include solid-wing/black shoulder (the black and brown
stripes on the wing are instead one solid colour), pied, white-eye (the ocelli
in a male's eye feathers have white spots instead of black), and silver pied (a
mostly white bird with small patches of colour). Colour variations include
white, purple, Buford bronze, opal, midnight, charcoal, jade, and taupe, as
well as the sex-linked colours purple, cameo, peach, and Sonja's Violeta.
Additional colour and pattern variations are first approved by the United
Peafowl Association to become officially recognised as a morph among breeders. Alternately coloured peafowl are born differently coloured than wild-type
peafowl, and though each colour is recognisable at hatch, their peachick
plumage does not necessarily match their adult plumage.
Occasionally, peafowl appears
with white plumage. Although albino peafowl does exist, this is
quite rare, and almost all white peafowl is not albinos; they have a genetic
condition called leucism, which causes pigment cells to fail to migrate
from the neural crest during development. Leucistic peafowl can produce pigment
but not deposit the pigment to their feathers, resulting in their blue-grey eye
colour and the complete lack of colouration in their plumage. Pied peafowl are
affected by partial leucism, where only some pigment cells fail to migrate,
resulting in birds that have colour but also have patches absent of all colour;
they, too, have blue-grey eyes. By contrast, true albino peafowl would have a
complete lack of melanin, resulting in irises that look red or pink.
Leucistic peachicks are born yellow and become fully white as they mature.
Iridescence
As with many birds, vibrant
iridescent plumage colours are not primarily pigments, but structural
colouration. Optical interference Bragg reflections, based on
regular, periodic nanostructures of the barbules (fibre-like
components) of the feathers, produce the peacock's colours. Slight changes to
the spacing of these barbules result in different colours. Brown feathers are a
mixture of red and blue: one colour is created by the periodic structure and
the other is created by a Fabry–Pérot interference peak from
reflections from the outer and inner boundaries. Such structural colouration
causes the iridescence of the peacock's hues. Interference effects depend on a light angle rather than actual pigments. Most commonly, during a courtship
display, the visiting female peahen will stop directly in front of the male
peacock - thus providing her with the ability to assess the male at 90° to the
surface of the feather. Then, the male will turn and display his feathers about
45° to the right of the sun’s azimuth which allows the sunlight to accentuate
the iridescence of his train. If the female chooses to interact with the male,
he will then turn to face her and shiver his train so as to begin the mating
process.
Evolution
and sexual selection
Charles Darwin suggested
in On the Origin of Species that the peafowl's plumage had
evolved through sexual selection. He expanded upon this in his second
book, The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex.
The sexual struggle is of two
kinds; in the one, it is between individuals of the same sex, generally the
males, in order to drive away or kill their rivals, the females remaining
passive; whilst in the other, the struggle is likewise between the individuals
of the same sex, in order to excite or charm those of the opposite sex,
generally, the females, which no longer remain passive, but select the more
agreeable partners.
Sexual selection is the
ability of male and female organisms to exert selective forces on each other
with regard to mating activity. The strongest driver of sexual selection
is gamete size. In general, eggs are bigger than sperm, and females produce
fewer gametes than males. This leads to eggs being a bigger investment, so females being choosy about the traits that will be passed on to their offspring
by males. The peahen's reproductive success and the likelihood of survival of
her chicks are partly dependent on the genotype of the mate. Females
generally have more to lose when mating with an inferior male due to their
gametes being more costly than the males.
Female
choice
Multiple hypotheses attempt to
explain the evolution of female choice. Some of these suggest direct benefits
to females, such as protection, shelter, or nuptial gifts that sway the female's
choice of mate. Another hypothesis is that females choose mates with good
genes. Males with more exaggerated secondary sexual characteristics, such as
bigger, brighter peacock trains, tend to have better genes in the peahen's
eyes. These better genes directly benefit her offspring, as well as her
fitness and reproductive success. Runaway selection also seeks to clarify the
evolution of the peacock's train. In runaway sexual selection, linked genes in
males and females code for sexually dimorphic traits in males, and preference
for those traits in females. The close spatial association of alleles for loci involved
in the train in males, and for preference for more exuberant trains in females,
on the chromosome (linkage disequilibrium) causes a positive feedback loop
that exaggerates both the male traits and the female preferences. Another
hypothesis is sensory bias, in which females have a preference for a trait in a
nonmating context that becomes transferred to mating. Multiple causalities for
the evolution of female choice is also possible.
Work concerning female
behaviour in many species of animals has sought to confirm Darwin's basic idea
of female preference for males with certain characteristics as a major force in
the evolution of species. Females have often been shown to distinguish
small differences between potential mates, and to prefer mating with
individuals bearing the most exaggerated characters. In some cases, those
males have been shown to be more healthy and vigorous, suggesting that the
ornaments serve as markers indicating the males' abilities to survive, and thus
their genetic qualities.
The peacock's train and
iridescent plumage are perhaps the best-known examples of traits believed to
have arisen through sexual selection, though with some controversy. Male
peafowl erects their trains to form a shimmering fan in their display to
females. Marion Petrie tested whether or not these displays signalled a male's
genetic quality by studying a feral population of peafowl in Whipsnade
Wildlife Park in southern England. The number of eyespots in the
train predicted a male's mating success. She was able to manipulate this
success by cutting the eyespots off some of the males' tails: females lost
interest in pruned males and became attracted to untrimmed ones. Males with
fewer eyespots, thus with lower mating success, suffered from greater
predation. She allowed females to mate with males with differing numbers
of eyespots and reared the offspring in a communal incubator to control for
differences in maternal care. Chicks fathered by more ornamented males weighed
more than those fathered by less ornamented males, an attribute generally
associated with a better survival rate in birds. These chicks were released into
the park and recaptured one year later. Those with heavily ornamented feathers
were better able to avoid predators and survive in natural conditions. Thus,
Petrie's work has shown correlations between tail ornamentation, mating
success, and increased survival ability in both the ornamented males and their
offspring.
Furthermore, peafowl and their
sexual characteristics have been used in the discussion of the causes for
sexual traits. Amotz Zahavi used the excessive tail plumes of male peafowls as
evidence for his "handicap principle". Since these trains are
likely to be deleterious to an individual's survival (as their brilliance makes
them more visible to predators and their length hinders escape from
danger), Zahavi argued that only the fittest males could survive the handicap
of a large train. Thus, a brilliant train serves as an honest indicator for
females that these highly ornamented males are good at surviving for other
reasons, so are preferable mates. This theory may be contrasted with Ronald
Fisher's theory (and Darwin's hypothesis) that male sexual traits are the
result of initially arbitrary aesthetic selection by females.
In contrast to Petrie's
findings, a seven-year Japanese study of free-ranging peafowl concluded that
female peafowl does not select mates solely on the basis of their trains. Mariko
Takahashi found no evidence that peahens preferred peacocks with more elaborate
trains (such as with more eyespots), a more symmetrical arrangement, or a
greater length. Takahashi determined that the peacock's train was not the
universal target of female mate choice, showed little variance across male
populations, and did not correlate with the male physiological condition. Adeline
Loyau and her colleagues responded that alternative and possibly central
explanations for these results had been overlooked. They concluded that the female choice might indeed vary in different ecological conditions.
Food
courtship theory
Merle Jacobs' food-courtship theory states that peahens are attracted to peacocks for the resemblance of
their eyespots to blueberries.
Natural
selection
It has been suggested that a
peacock's train, loud call, and fearless behaviour have been formed by natural
selection (not sexual selection), and served as an aposematic display
to intimidate predators and rivals.
Plumage
colours as attractants
A peacock's copulation success
rate depends on the colours of his eyespots (ocelli) and the angle at
which they are displayed. The angle at which the ocelli are displayed during
courtship is more important in a peahen's choice of males than train size or the number of ocelli. Peahens pay careful attention to the different parts of
a peacock's train during his display. The lower train is usually evaluated
during close-up courtship, while the upper train is more of a long-distance
attraction signal. Actions such as train rattling and wing shaking also kept
the peahens' attention.
Redundant
signal hypothesis
Although an intricate display
catches a peahen's attention, the redundant signal hypothesis also plays a
crucial role in keeping this attention on the peacock's display. The redundant
signal hypothesis explains that whilst each signal that a male project is
about the same quality, the addition of multiple signals enhances the
reliability of that mate. This idea also suggests that the success of multiple
signalling is not only due to the repetitiveness of the signal but also of
multiple receivers of the signal. In the peacock species, males congregate a
communal display during the breeding season and the peahens observe. Peacocks first
defend their territory through intra-sexual behaviour, defending their areas
from intruders. They fight for areas within the congregation to display a
strong front for the peahens. Central positions are usually taken by older,
dominant males, which influences mating success. Certain morphological and
behavioural traits come into play during inter and intra-sexual selection, which
include train length for territory acquisition and visual and vocal displays
involved in mate choice by peahens.
Vocalisation
In courtship, vocalisation
stands to be a primary way for peacocks to attract peahens. Some studies
suggest that the intricacy of the "song" produced by displaying
peacocks proved to be impressive to peafowl. Singing in peacocks usually occurs
just before, just after, or sometimes during copulation.
Plumage change
Peafowl
are omnivores and eat mostly plants, flower petals, seed heads, insects
and other arthropods, reptiles, and amphibians. Wild peafowl
looks for their food scratching around in leaf litter either early in the
morning or at dusk. They retreat to the shade and security of the woods for the
hottest portion of the day. These birds are not picky and will eat almost
anything they can fit in their beak and digest. They actively hunt insects like
ants, crickets and termites; millipedes; and other arthropods and
small mammals. Indian peafowl also eats small snakes.
Domesticated peafowl may also eat bread and cracked grain such as oats and corn, cheese, cooked rice and sometimes cat food. It has been noticed by keepers that peafowl enjoy protein-rich food including larvae that infest granaries, different kinds of meat and fruit, as well as vegetables including dark leafy greens, broccoli, carrots, beans, beets, and peas.
0 Comments